Reviews with all double blind testing?


In the July, 2005 issue of Stereophile, John Atkinson discusses his debate with Arnold Krueger, who Atkinson suggest fundamentally wants only double blind testing of all products in the name of science. Atkinson goes on to discuss his early advocacy of such methodology and his realization that the conclusion that all amps sound the same, as the result of such testing, proved incorrect in the long run. Atkinson’s double blind test involved listening to three amps, so it apparently was not the typical different or the same comparison advocated by those advocating blind testing.

I have been party to three blind testings and several “shootouts,” which were not blind tests and thus resulted in each component having advocates as everyone knew which was playing. None of these ever resulted in a consensus. Two of the three db tests were same or different comparisons. Neither of these resulted in a conclusion that people could consistently hear a difference. One was a comparison of about six preamps. Here there was a substantial consensus that the Bozak preamp surpassed more expensive preamps with many designers of those preamps involved in the listening. In both cases there were individuals that were at odds with the overall conclusion, and in no case were those involved a random sample. In all cases there were no more than 25 people involved.

I have never heard of an instance where “same versus different” methodology ever concluded that there was a difference, but apparently comparisons of multiple amps and preamps, etc. can result in one being generally preferred. I suspect, however, that those advocating db, mean only “same versus different” methodology. Do the advocates of db really expect that the outcome will always be that people can hear no difference? If so, is it the conclusion that underlies their advocacy rather than the supposedly scientific basis for db? Some advocates claim that were there a db test that found people capable of hearing a difference that they would no longer be critical, but is this sincere?

Atkinson puts it in terms of the double blind test advocates want to be right rather than happy, while their opponents would rather be happy than right.

Tests of statistical significance also get involved here as some people can hear a difference, but if they are insufficient in number to achieve statistical significance, then proponents say we must accept the null hypothesis that there is no audible difference. This is all invalid as the samples are never random samples and seldom, if ever, of a substantial size. Since the tests only apply to random samples and statistical significance is greatly enhanced with large samples, nothing in the typical db test works to yield the result that people can hear a difference. This would suggest that the conclusion and not the methodology or a commitment to “science” is the real purpose.

Without db testing, the advocates suggest those who hear a difference are deluding themselves, the placebo effect. But were we to use db but other than the same/different technique and people consistently choose the same component, would we not conclude that they are not delusional? This would test another hypothesis that some can hear better.

I am probably like most subjectivists, as I really do not care what the outcomes of db testing might be. I buy components that I can afford and that satisfy my ears as realistic. Certainly some products satisfy the ears of more people, and sometimes these are not the positively reviewed or heavily advertised products. Again it strikes me, at least, that this should not happen in the world that the objectivists see. They see the world as full of greedy charlatans who use advertising to sell expensive items which are no better than much cheaper ones.

Since my occupation is as a professor and scientist, some among the advocates of double blind might question my commitment to science. My experience with same/different double blind experiments suggest to me a flawed methodology. A double blind multiple component design, especially with a hypothesis that some people are better able to hear a difference, would be more pleasing to me, but even here, I do not think anyone would buy on the basis of such experiments.

To use Atkinson’s phrase, I am generally happy and don’t care if the objectivists think I am right. I suspect they have to have all of us say they are right before they can be happy. Well tough luck, guys. I cannot imagine anything more boring than consistent findings of no difference among wires and components, when I know that to be untrue. Oh, and I have ordered additional Intelligent Chips. My, I am a delusional fool!
tbg
For those embracing DBT as simple self-endorsement, I am dismissive.

No objectivists of my acquaintance (and I am acquainted with some fairly prominent ones), "embrace DBT as simple self-endorsement." A number of them, myself included, were subjectivists until we heard something that just didn't make sense to us. I know of one guy (whose name you would recognize) who was switching between two components and had zeroed in on what he was sure were the audible differences between them. Then he discovered that the switch wasn't working! He'd been listening to the same component the whole time, and the differences, while quite "obvious," turned out to be imaginary. He compared them again, blind this time, and couldn't hear a difference. He stopped doing sighted comparisons that day.

Research psychologists did not adopt blind testing because it gave them the results they wanted. They adopted it because it was the only way to get reliable results at all. Audio experts who rely on blind testing do so for the same reason.

Final thought: No one has to use blind comparisons if they don't want to. (Truth be told, while I've done a few, I certainly don't use them when I'm shopping for audio equipment.) Maybe that supertweeter really doesn't make a difference, but if you think it does, and you're happy with it, that's just fine. Just don't get into a technical argument with those guys from NHK!
Tbg...I also can "hear" the effect of tweeters/supertweeters operating well above the measured bandwidth of my 67-year old ears. (I first noticed this general effect, at higher frequencies, when I was much younger). My explanation is that the ear senses RATE-of-change of pressure, as well as change of pressure. The high rate of change of a 20 KHz signal can be sensed, even if the smoothly changing pressure of a 14KHz signal is inaudible. The experience we share is common. Have you heard any other explanation?
ELdartford sez
the ear senses RATE-of-change of pressure (...)Have you heard any other explanation
Well, 1) about 20yrs ago a french prof (forgot the name) claimed findings that the bones contribute to our perception of very high frequencies. 2) There seems to be a case for the interaural mechanism working together -- not ONE ear alone, but both being excited.

OTOH, it's also been established that the audibility of PURE tones diminishes with age in the higher frequencies. So here, we're talking about "sound in context": i.e. say, harmonics of an instrument -- where the fundamental & certain harmonics are well within our pure tone hearing range and some of the related info is outside an individual's "official" (pure tone) audible range.

The strange thing is that our ears work as a low pass; so, some people speculate that it's the COMMON interaural excitation that does the trick...
For this to happen (let's ignore the possible contribution of the bone structure for now) would'nt it mean that our interaural "mechanism" is situated in the DIRECT path (sweet spot) of those frequencies (remember, our acuity falls dramatically, ~20-30db, up there). If so, then moving our head slightly would eliminate this perception.

So, let's assume a super high frequency transducer with excellent dispersion characteristics and thereby eliminate the need for that narrow sweet spot (a Murata is quite good, btw).

It is my contention (but I have no concrete evidence) that three things are happening in conjunction:
a) the high frequency sound is loud enough to overcome our reduced acuity up high (at -60db perception our ear would basically reject it)
b) the sounds in our "official" audible frequency range are rendered more palpable (for wont of a better word) because the super transducer's distortion points (upper resonance) have moved very far away (it's ~100kHz for a Murata) -- hence "perception" of positive effects. This still relates to our "official" range of hearing.

b) there is a combined excitation of aural and other, structural, mechanisms that indicate the presence of high frequencies -- that we cannot, however, qualify or explain (our hearing is a defense and guidance mechanism geared towards perceiving and locating).
Even at B there is a dilemma: in a small experiment in France some subjects were asked to put one ear close to a super tweet and declare whether they perceive anything. Inconclusive (some did, some didn't, no pattern. BTW, I did a similar thing & did perceive energy or lack of it with some DELAY however when the tweet STOPPED producing sound -- joining Eldartford's idea).
Subjects were then asked to move away from the transducer & listen normally (stereo), just by casually sitting on a couch in front of the speakers as one would do at home. Everyone "heard" the supertweet playing. Amazingly, only the s-tweet was connected (at 16kHz -- very high up for sound out of other context).
I find this fascinating.
Gregm, I do not know how many out there experienced the Murata demonstration at CES 2004, but it was a great deal like what you describe. Initially, the speakers played a passage. Then the super tweeters were used and the passage replayed. The ten people in the audience all expressed a preference for the use of the super tweeters. There was much conversation but ultimately someone asked to hear the super tweeter only. The demonstrator said, we already were hearing it.

When we all refocused on the sound, all that we could hear was an occasional spit, tiz, snap. There was no music at all. The Muratas come in at 15k Hz. I left and dragged several friends back for a second demonstration with exactly the same results.

Would there be any benefit to having this done single or double blind? I don't think so. Do we need to have an understanding for how we hear such high frequency information, without which it might be a placebo or Hawthorne Electric phenomenon? I don't.

But this experience is quite at odds with the article that Pabelson cited. What is going on? I certainly don't know, save to suggest that there is a difference in what is being asked of subjects in the two tests.
I teach a course on the philosophy of color and color perception. One of the things I do is show color chips that are pairwise indistinguishable. I show a green chip together with another green chip that is indistinguishable. Then, I take away the first chip and show a third green chip that is indistinguishable from the second. And then I toss the second chip and introduce a fourth chip, indistinguishable from the third. At this point, I bring back the first green chip and compare it with the fourth. The fourth chip now looks bluish by contrast, and is easily distinguished from the original. How does that happen? We don't notice tiny differences, but they add up to noticable differences. We can be walked, step-wise, from any color to any other color without ever noticing a difference, provided our steps are small enough!

Same for sound, I bet. That's why I don't understand the obsession with pair-wise double-blind testing of individual components. Comparing two amps, alone, may not yield a discriminable difference. Likewise, two preamps might be pairwise indiscriminable. But the amp-pre-amp combos (there will be four possibilities) may be *noticably* different from one another. I bet this happens, but the tests are all about isolating one component and distinguishing it from a competitor, which is exactly wrong!

The same goes for wire and cable. It may be difficult to discern the result of swapping out one standard power cord or set of ic's or speaker cables. But replace all of them together and then test the completely upgraded set against the stock setup and see what you've got. At least, I'd love to see double-blind testing that is holistic like this. I'd take the results very seriously.

From the holistic tests, you can work backward to see what is contributing to good sound, just as you can eventually align all color chips in the proper order, if presented with the whole lot of them. But what needs to be compared in the first place are large chunks of the system. Even if amp/pre-amp combos couldn't be distinguished, perhaps amp/pre-amp combos with different cabling could be (even though none of the three elements used distinguishable products!). I want to see this done. Double blind.

In short: unnoticable difference add up to *very* noticable differences. Why this non-additive nature of comparison isn't at the forefront of the subjectivist/objectivist debate is a complete mystery to me.

-Troy